Thomas A. Ban
Neuropsychopharmacology in Historical Perspective
Education in the Field in the Post-Neuropsychopharmacology Era.

 

Thomas A. Ban: Neuropsychopharmacology and the Forgotten Language of Psychiatry

(Collated Bulletin 2)

 

Thomas A, Ban           November 14, 2013              essay   

Barry Blackwell          January 2, 2014                   comment

Thomas A, Ban           January 23, 2014                 reply

Jose de Leon              May 22, 2014                       comment

Thomas A. Ban           December 4, 2014                reply

Hector Warnes           March 26, 2015                     comment

Thomas A. Ban           May 14, 2015                        reply   

Hector Warnes           July 23, 2015                        response

Thomas A. Ban           July 30, 2015                        response

 

Thomas A. Ban: Neuropsychopharmacology and the Forgotten Language of Psychiatry

Madness: From Psychiatry to Neuronology via Neuropsychopharmacology

 

Contents

 

From Neurosis to Psychosis                                                                        

Introduction of Conditioned Reflex                                                            

Development of the Language of Psychiatry                                                                                   Introduction of Psychopathology                                                              

Introduction of Nosology                                                                           

Forgetting the Language of Psychiatry                                                                 

Rediscovering the Language of Psychiatry                                                            

Revival of Nosology                                                                                  

                        Diagnostic Criteria of Research                                                          

                        Composite Diagnostic Evaluation System                                    

             Revival of Psychopathology                                                                     

                        Nosologic Homotyping                                                                   

The Conditioned Reflex Revisited                                                                        

From Psychiatry to Neuronology                                                                           

References                                                                                                             

 

From Neurosis to Psychosis

          

         Madness may be as old as mankind (Porter 2002). Yet, development that led to the birth of psychiatry, the discipline that deals with “madness,” began only in the late 18th century. It was triggered by: (1) William Cullen’s (1777) introduction of the term “neurosis,” in his First Lines of Physic, for a class of disease he believed were diseases of the “nerves” and (2) his classifying the “vesanias” that included the various forms of madness, as one of the four “orders” of the “neuroses” {1} (Littre 1877).  

         Cullen was an influential professor of medicine and physics at the University of Edinburgh, Scotland and his classification attracted attention in Continental Europe and the United States. Hence, his classifying “madness” as diseases of the “nerves” could not be dismissed by the “mentalists” (referred to by some as “German Romanticists”), powerful group of physicians at the time that believed that “insanity” was an affliction of the “mind” (Pichot 1983; Shorter 2005).

         To shift emphasis from the nerves (brain) back to the mind (psyche) in the understanding of “madness,” the term “Psychiaterie” was introduced in 1808 by Johann Christian Reil, the professor of medicine in Halle (Germany). It was adopted and modified to “Psychiatrie” by Johann Christian Heinroth, the professor of medicine in Leipzig (Germany).

         It was through Heinroth’s influential Textbook on the Disturbances of Psychic Life, published in 1818, that the term “psychiatry” spread around the world (Pichot 1983).

         The “mentalists” profoundly affected the language and thinking- of “psychiatry” with a long-lasting effect.

         In 1845 Ernst Feuchtersleben, the dean of medicine at the University of Vienna (Austria), not a mentalist himself, had adopted the “mentalist” term “psychosis” in his Textbook of Medical Psychology for patients with “madness” who qualified for the “vesanias“ in Cullen’s classification. His separation of patients with “madness,” i.e., patients with “psychosis” from the other patients with “neurosis,” marks the separation of “psychiatry” from “neurology.” Within Feuchersleben’s frame of reference: ”Every mental disorder implies a disease of the nervous system, but not every defect of the nervous system is accompanied by mental disorder” (Pichot 1983).

 

Introduction of terms that set the stage for the development of psychiatry

  

William Cullen

Edinburgh (Scotland)

Neurosis

1777

 

Johann Christian Reil

Halle

Psychaterie

1808

 

Johann Christian Heinroth

Leipzig

Psychiatrie

1818

 

Ernst Feuchtersleben

Vienna (Austria)

Psychosis

1845

 

Introduction of Conditioned Réflex

         In the same year, 1845, that Feuchtersleben’s textbook appeared, Wilhelm Griesinger published his treatise, The Pathology and Therapy of Psychic Illnesses. For Griesinger, psychiatry was part of the natural sciences; mental activity was nervous activity and mental pathology was a symptom of brain disease.

         Stimulated by Sir Charles Bell’s (1811) discovery and François Magendie’s (1822) recognition of the importance of the “reflex arc” that links sensory input with motor output in the functioning of the nervous system (spinal cord), Griesinger  (1843) was first to perceive mental activity as “reflex” activity. He was also the first to describe in 1843 “psychic reflex actions” (psychische Reflexactionen).

         By adopting the “reflex” as he elementary unit of “mental activity” in 1843, Griesinger set the stage for the development of psychiatry as a medical discipline. Twenty years later, in 1863, Ivan Mihailovich Sechenov, a Russian physiologist, who studied “nervous inhibition” in the central nervous system of the frog in Claude Bernard’s laboratory in  Paris, elaborated on Griesinger’s descriptions. In his monograph, Reflexes of the Brain, he concluded that all activity, including the “psychological” in the brain, is reflex and as such follows fixed laws determinable by investigation (Sechenov 1935; Wells 1956).underpinning of the “psychic reflex” was established in the late 19th century by Camillo Golgi (1874), an Italian histologist, who described multi-polar (Golgi) cells in the “olfactory bulb” with the employment of silver staining; Santiago Ramon y Cajal (1894), a Spanish histologist, who established that the “neuron” is the morphological and functional unit of the nervous system; and Sir Charles Sherrington (1906), an English physiologist, who demonstrated that the “synapse” is the functional site of transmission from one neuron to another.

 

Discoveries that provided the structural foundation of brain functioning

 

Camillo Golgi

Italy

1874

multipolar cells in olfactory bulb

 

Ramon y Cajal

Spain

1883

multipolar cells

in olfactory bulb

 

Charles Sherrington

England

1906

synapse

functional site of transmission

 

         Carl Wernicke (1881-1883), the professor of neurology and psychiatry in Breslau (Germany at the time), in the late 19th century, adopted Griesinger’s view that mental activity is “reflex” activity and classified “psychoses,” i.e., psychiatric diseases, on the basis of “hyper-functioning,” “hypo-functioning or “para-functioning” in the “psycho-sensory,” “intra-psychic” (trans-cortical) and/or “psychomotor” components (paths, phases) of the “psychic reflex” (Franzek 1990).       

         The “psychic reflex” became central in the research of Ivan Petrovich Pavlov, a Russian physiologist and recipient of the Nobel Prize in 1904 for the discovery of the nervous regulation of the heart. His interest in the “psychic reflex” was triggered by his (and others) observation that “sham feeding” produced gastric secretion in a dog (Pavlov 1906). To study this phenomenon he developed a behavioural method for the detection and measurement of salivary secretion in chronic experiments in dogs with a surgical fistula in their parotid glands. With the employment of this method Pavlov discovered that any sensory stimulus (ringing of a bell in the original experiments) by repeated (preceding) coincidence with a specific stimulus for a particular reflex (food in the mouth for salivary secretion in the original experiments) became a signal for the specific stimulus, i.e., ringing of a bell became a signal for eliciting salivary secretion for which food in the mouth was the only signal before.

         Pavlov could explain his findings only by assuming the opening of a new, formerly non-operating path in the brain with each newly formed conditional  reflex (CR), a term he coined. Hence, he postulated that “psychic activity,” as salivating to the ringing of a bell, translates into changes in the processing of sensory signals in the cerebral cortex. He also rendered the “psychic reflex,” i.e., the changes in the processing of sensory signals in the brain, accessible to study with a behavioural method he developed in his laboratory. To distinguish the behaviourally indistinguishable acquired reflex from the innate reflex, introduced the term conditional reflex (CR) for the former and the term unconditional reflex (UR) for the latter.

         In the first two decades of the 20th century Pavlov with his associates established that the brain of some mammals has built-in potential to form, i.e., acquire CRs (CR acquisition); extinguish-inhibit, acquired CRs (CR extinction); and “disinhibit” extinguished CRs (CR disinhibition). They also revealed that before the CR becomes restricted to a particular conditional stimulus (CS) by differentiating the CS from other stimuli (CR differentiation), it becomes “generalized” in a manner that transiently, any qualitatively similar stimulus to the CS can elicit the CR (CR generalization). Based on two assumed ongoing “elementary basic processes” in the brain, “excitation” and “inhibition”, which become manifest behaviourally in CR acquisition and CR extinction, the brain has the potential for differentiation (CR differentiation), by reinforcing one stimulus (positive stimulus) and not-reinforcing another (negative stimulus), and CR reversal, by shifting reinforcement from the positive to the negative stimulus. Furthermore, the brain also has the potential to delay the onset of the CR (CR delay or retardation) and to form secondary CRs or chains of CRs built on established CRs {2} (Ban 1964; Gantt 1948; Pavlov 1927).

 

Ivan Petrovich Pavlov’s contributions:

 

DISCOVERED that any sensory stimulus can become a signal for a specific sensory stimulus if repeatedly coincides (preceding coincidence) with the specific stimulus;

EXPLAINED his finding by assuming opening of new, formerly non-operating path in the brain;

HYPOTHESIZED that “psychic activity” is based on changes in the processing of sensory signals in the brain;

REPLACED the term “psychic reflex” with the term “conditioned reflex” (CR);

RENDERED the built-in potential of the brain for processing signals accessible to study via CR functions:

acquisition

extinction

disinhibition

generalization

differentiation

reversal

retardation

secondary CR formation

CR chain formation

 

         In the early 1930s Pavlov with his associates extended their research from animal to man and revealed that the human brain has the potential to use a corresponding verbal signal of a sensory stimulus as a signal to elicit the CR. Since verbal signals are built on sensory signals, Pavlov referred to CR activity with the use of words as second signal system activity, and CR activity with the use of sensory signals as first signal system activity. Furthermore, since CR activity in both, the first and the second signal systems are built on UR activity, he distinguished between lower, UR based nervous activity, and higher, CR based nervous activity.

         In Pavlov’s frame of reference mental functioning is higher nervous activity, and mental pathology is an expression of abnormal functioning in the second signal system.  The findings that CRs to verbal signals suppress CRs to sensory stimuli, and CRs to sensory stimuli, suppress URs indicate that human behaviour is dominated by verbal signals (Ban 1966; Bykov 1957; Ivanov-Smolensky 1954; Wells 1956). Since CRs in the first and the second signal systems are based on the same built in potential of the brain for CR functions, CR parameters, such as CR acquisition, CR extinction, generalization, differentiation, secondary CR formation,  etc., provide an indirect means for the study of normal and abnormal mental functioning. Hence, if abnormal CR functions could be linked to psychopathology and its underlying abnormality, CR parameters would provide a bridge between the “language” of mental pathology and the “language” of pathological brain functioning.  

 

Potentials of the Human Brain that provides the basis for mental activity

1.to use corresponding word of a sensory CS as a signal to elicit CR;

2.CRs to sensory signals suppress URs and CRs to verbal signals suppress CRs to sensory signals;

3.CRs in the first (sensory) and CRs in the second (verbal) signal systems are based on the same built in potential of the brain for CR functions;

4.operates prevailingly with CRs, dominated by CRs to  verbal signals;

5.mental pathology is an expression of an abnormality in second signal system functioning;

6.CR parameters such as CR acquisition, CR extinction, etc., provide a means for the study of normal and abnormal functioning in both the first and the second signal systems;

7.if abnormal CR functions could be linked to psychopathology and its underlying pathophysiology, CR parameters could serve as a bridge between the language of psychiatry and the language of brain functioning.

 

Development of the Language of Psychiatry

 

Introduction of Psychopathology

         It was Galen (131-201) first to recognize that “symptoms” follow disease as shadow its substance (Garrison 1929). Yet, development of “psychopathology,” the “language of psychiatry,” began only in the mid-19th century in the course of early attempts to differentiate sub-populations within “insanity.”

         The term psychopathology first appeared in the psychiatric literature in 1845 in Feuchtersleben’s textbook, the same book in which the term ”psychosis” was adopted; throughout the second half of the 19th century the term was used as a synonym for psychiatry.

         During the 19th century the vocabulary of psychopathology steadily grew. Esquirol’s (1838) divided false perceptions into “illusions” (distortion or misinterpretation of real perception) and hallucinations (perceptual experiences without corresponding stimuli in the environment); Griesinger’s (1845) distinguished between “pale (pseudo) hallucinations” (that appear in the inner subjective space and can be controlled voluntarily) and  “true (real) hallucinations” (usually referred to hallucinations” simply); Wernicke (1881) separated  “dysmnesia” (memory impairment) from “dementia”  (personality deterioration).

         Psychopathology became a discipline to provide a foundation for psychiatry in the early years of the 20th century. Instrumental to this development was Karl Jaspers (1910, 1913) observation that in different psychiatric disease patients’ process (in their brain) and consequently perceive the same “content” (information) in different “forms.” His recognition of the relationship between the ”forms” in which information (“content”) is perceived by patients  and their  illness, led to the birth of “phenomenological psychopathology” (phenomenology), the branch of psychopathology that deals with “abnormal subjective experiences of individual psychic life.” It also led to his separation of “psychiatric disease process,” displayed by “abnormal forms of experiences,” from “abnormal personality development,” displayed by behaviour that deviates from the statistical norm.    

         For the “phenomenologist,” it is not the subject matter, the information, (“content”) the patient talks about, but how (“form”) the patient talks, and it is not the “somatic (hypochondriacal) complaints” (“contents), but the form, how these complaints are experienced, as “bodily hallucinations” (somatic experiences without corresponding stimuli in the environment), “obsessive ideas” (ideas that persist against one’s will), “hypochondriacal delusions” (false beliefs based on a priori evidence) that is relevant to diagnosis (Fish 1967; Taylor 1981). Even in case of “delusions,” a “content disorder of thinking” that signals the presence of an ongoing psychiatric disease (“psychosis”), it is not the “content“ of the “delusions,”  such as “delusions of reference,” “delusions of love,” “delusions of persecution,” etc.,   but the “form” in which the “delusion” appears,  i.e., a “sudden delusional idea” (a delusional idea that appears to be fully formed),  a “delusional perception” (a delusional meaning  attributed to a normally perceived object),  that is relevant to the characteristic abnormality of the processing of signals by the  brain that differentiates one psychiatric disease (process) from another (Guy and Ban 1982;  Hamilton 1985).

         It was on the basis of  “phenomenological analyses” that Kurt Schneider (1920, 1950), distinguished between “vital depression,”  a disease, from the “other depressions,” and separated “personality disorders,” displayed in “abnormal variations of psychic life,” the subject matter of “abnormal psychology,” from “psychoses” (mental disorders), displayed in “abnormal forms of experiences,” the subject matter of “psychiatry.”

         During the years from 1918 to 1933 a group of psychiatrists that included Hans Gruhle and Wilhelm Mayer-Gross, in Kurt Wilmanns’ department of psychiatry at Heidelberg University in Germany, spearheaded “phenomenological analyses” in psychiatric patients (Shorter 2005). Their effort has yielded a vocabulary that includes distinct words (symptoms) from pathologies of “symbolization,” such as “condensation” (combining diverse ideas into one concept) and “onematopoesis” (building new phrases in which the usual language conventions are not observed), to pathologies of “psychomotility,” such as “ambitendency” (the presence of opposite tendencies to action) and “parakinesis” (qualitatively abnormal movements). In  “phenomenology,” “dysphoria,” the negative pole of “vital emotions,” is distinguished from “dysthymia,” the negative pole of mood,  “psychomotor retardation,” the experience of a spontaneous slowing down of motor activity, is distinguished from “psychomotor inhibition,” the experience of slowed down motor activity, etc.

         Furthermore, by linking the terms that identify the different abnormalities to psychiatric diagnoses in use at the time, e.g., “tangential thinking,” characterized by talking past and around the point, with the “schizophrenias,” “circumstantial thinking,” characterized by overbearing elaboration on insignificant details without losing track, with the “dementias” and “rumination,” characterized by endless repetition of unpleasant thoughts, with “depressions,” the Heidelberg group set the foundation of a language for

psychiatry.

 

Heidelberg School of Psychiatry

(1918-1933)

 

Phenomenological Analysis

 

VOCABULARY

for

language of psychiatry

 

WORDS

from pathologies of “symbolization” (“condensation”,  “onematopoesis”)

to

pathologies of “psychomotility” (“ambitendency”, parakinesis”)

 

DISTINCTIONS

between

“dysphoria”  vs  “dysthymia,”

“psychomotor retardation” vs “psychomotor inhibition”

 

SYMPTOMS  & DIAGNOSES

tangential thinking  - schizophrenias

circumstantial thinking – dementias

rumination - depressions

 

Introduction of Nosology

         The vocabulary of “psychopathology” that deals with cross-sectional features of disease, was extended to include the vocabulary of “psychiatric nosology” for describing psychiatric disease in its “dynamic totality” from “onset” through “course” to “outcome” (Ban 1987).

         The two disciplines, “psychopathology” and “psychiatric nosology,” are intrinsically connected; psychopathology deals with symptoms, i.e., abnormal subjective experiences (“phenomenology”) and signs, i.e., “objective performance changes” (“performance psychology”), whereas ”nosology” deals with the synthesis of “disease entities” from symptoms and signs, and classification of the  diseases  synthesized (Jaspers 1962). While classifications provide names (denominations) and descriptions of disease (qualifications), nosology provides the methodology “how” diseases and classification of diseases are derived (Ban 2000).  

         The term “nosology” first appeared in 1743 in Robert James’ Medical Dictionary. Twenty-five years later in 1768 it reappeared in the title of Francois Boissier de Sauvages‘ Nosologia Methodica.

         In his treatise, de Sauvages stipulates that a disease should be defined by the enumeration of symptoms that suffice to recognize it and distinguish it from others (diseases), and a classification should be devised in a manner that it should allow the attribution of each patient to one and only one class. Thus, the emphasis in disease is that each patient with the same disease displays the same symptoms and thereby is different in terms of symptoms from patients with any other disease, whereas the emphasis in a class is on shared characteristics of diseases in terms of “course” and “outcome” regardless of differences in symptomatic expressions.

         One year after the publication of de Sauvages’ treatise, the term, “nosology” was adopted also by William Cullen (1769) in the title of his Synopsis Nosologiae Methodicae.           In his First Linse of  the Practice of Physics, published three years later, Cullen (1772) divided “madness” into four  classes of disease:  “amentia“ (“imbecility of judgment, by which people do not perceive, or do not remember the relation of things”),  “melancholia” (“partial madness’ without dyspepsia, varying according to the different subjects concerning which the person raves”), “mania” (“universal madness”)  and “oneroidynia” (violent and troublesome imagination in time of sleep”) (Menninger, Mayman and Pruyser 1968).  His separation of “universal” (total) from “partial” madness, on the basis  of “totality” of mental pathology,  was to dominate  classifications of insanity in the 19th century from Philippe Pinel’s (1798) and Jean-Étienne Dominique Esquirol’s (1838) in France, who distinguished between “mania” (universal insanity) and the “monomanias” (partial insanities), to Karl Kahlbaum’s (1863) in Germany, who distinguished between the  “vesanias” (total-universal insanities) and the  “vecordias” (partial insanities).

         The separation of “universal” from “partial” madness during the second half of the 19th century was based on pervasiveness of pathology manifested by “deterioration of personality” and/or “absence of insight.” Thus, Ernest-Charles Lasègue’s (1852) diagnostic concept of “persecutory delusional psychosis,” the predecessor of Kahlbaum’s (1874) diagnostic concept of ”paranoia,” was referred to as “partial insanity”  because  of a lack of “personality deterioration” and Carl Friedrich Otto Westphal’s (1878) diagnostic concept of “obsessive states” (Zwangsvorstellungen), the predecessor of the diagnostic concept of “obsessive-compulsive neurosis,” was referred to as “abortive insanity,” a form of “partial insanity,” because patients had “insight” about the pathological nature of their condition. 

         The distinction between “universal” and “partial” madness was lingering on during the first six decades of the 20th century. In 1913, in the 8th edition of his textbook, Emil Kraepelin used the distinction between “universal” and “partial madness” for the separation of “paranoia” and the “paraphrenias” (partial insanities) from “dementia praecox” (total insanity); and in 1957 in his Classification of Endogenous Psychoses, Karl Leonhard used the distinction for the separation of “pure mania” and “pure melancholia (total insanities) from the “pure euphorias” and “pure depressions.”  

         Prior to Boissier de Sauvages and Cullen, in the late 17th century, Thomas Sydenham’s conceptualized disease as a “process” with a “natural history of its own” that “runs a regular and predictable course” (Ban 2000). Yet, in psychiatry it was only about 200 years later that Jean-Pierre Falret (1854) in the 1850s, identified a disease, fôlie circulaire, the predecessor of “manic-depressive insanity,” on the basis of its “temporal characteristics.”  It was also Falret first in the mid-1860s to stipulate that “a natural form of psychiatric illness implies a well-defined predictable course” and vice versa, “a well- defined predictable course presupposes the existence of a natural species of disease with a specified pattern of development” (Pichot 1983). A similar notion to Falret’s was expressed in 1874 by Karl Ludwig Kahlbaum in his “nosological postulate.”  It was also  Sydenham’s concept of disease that led Emil Kraepelin to replace his syndromic classification in the 4th edition of his textbook, published in 1894 with a disease-oriented classification in the 5th edition (1896).

         Kraepelin’s (1899) division (“dichotomy”) of the “endogenous psychoses” on the basis of “temporal characteristics,” i.e., “course” and “outcome,” in the 6th edition of his textbook into “manic depressive insanity,” a disease that follows an episodic course with full remission between episodes and “dementia praecox”, a disease that follows a continuous deteriorating course, led to a re-evaluation of psychiatric diagnoses and classifications, and especially (but not only) of the classification of diseases that Paul Julius Möbius (1893, 1900) referred to as “endogenous psychoses.” In the course of this re-evaluation both, diseases with and episodic course and diseases with a continuous course were divided into several forms. Within the diseases with an episodic course with full remission between episodes, diseases which manifest in “attacks” (that last from minutes to hours), as Lasègue’s (1877) “mental vertigo,” or  in “phases” (that last from days to years), as Edna Neele’s (1949) “phasic psychose,” were distinguished from diseases characterized by an episodic course without full remission between episodes which manifest in “thrusts” (“shifts”), as Bleuler’s schizophrenias (1911). And within the diseases with “continuous course,” diseases which  lead to highly  differentiated “end-states,” as Leonhard’s (1936) “defect (referred to later as ‘systematic’) schizophrenias” in the “endogenous psychoses,” were distinguished from diseases which lead to a dedifferentiated “terminal state,” as Alzheimers’ (1907)  disease (Ban 2000).  

         Kraepelin’s classification of the “endogenous psychoses” was first re-evaluated in the 1920s by Karl Kleist (1921, 1923, 1928); then by Karl Leonhard (1957) in the 1950s with the incorporation of some of Kleist’s contributions, e.g., the diagnostic concept of “cycloid psychoses.”

         In his re-evaluation, Leonhard employed Neele’s (1949) “polarity” and Wernicke’s (1881, 1899) “mental structure” in classifying patients.   With the employment of “polarity” he divided the population  already separated by “course” and “outcome” into “bipolar” and “unipolar diseases” and separated within both, several subpopulations on the basis of the site of the dominant psychopathology, i.e.,  the afferent-cognitive (“psychosensory”), central-affective (“intrapsychic”), or efferent-motor (“psychomotor”) component, in  Wernicke’s “mental structure.”  

         In Leonhard’s (1957) classification, “bipolar diseases” are characterized by a continuously changing “polymorph” (multiform) disease picture with a potential to display both extremes in mood, thinking, emotions and/or motility, whereas “unipolar (monopolar) diseases” are characterized by a consistent, unchanging, “monomorph” (simple, also referred to as pure) disease picture with no variation of mood, thinking, emotions and/or motility.

         On the basis of “polarity, Leonhard splits Kraepelin’s “dementia praecox,” (Bleuler’s (1911) “schizophrenias”, into two classes of disease: “(bipolar) unsystematic (non-systematic)  schizophrenias” and “(unipolar) systematic schizophrenias,” and   on the basis of Wernicke’s “mental structure” he divides “unsystematic schizophrenias” into three diseases, i.e., “cataphasia,” “affect-laden paraphrenia” and “periodic catatonia.” Similarly, he divides the “systematic schizophrenias” into three groups of diseases, i.e., “paraphrenias” (with six psychopathology-based sub-forms), “hebephrenias” (with four psychopathology-based sub-forms) and “catatonias” (with six psychopathology-based sub-forms).

         He also splits, on the basis of “polarity,” “manic depressive insanity” into “(bipolar) manic depressive disease” and “(unipolar) phasic psychoses,” and with consideration of Wernicke’s “mental structure,” he separates from “manic depressive disease” the “cycloid psychoses,” and divides the “cycloid psychoses,” into “excited-inhibited confusion psychosis,” “anxiety-happiness psychosis” and “hyperkinetic-akinetic motility psychosis.”

         Furthermore, on the basis “totality”, the organizing principle introduced by Cullen (1769), he separates “pure mania” and “pure melancholia” from the “pure euphorias” and “pure depressions,” each displayed in five distinct psychopathology-based forms.

         Within the “bipolar-polymorph” diseases the signal difference between “manic depressive disease” and the “cycloid psychoses” is that in “manic depressive disease” the “polarity” is prevailingly in mood, whereas in the “cycloid psychoses” the “polarity” is prevailingly in thinking (“excited-inhibited confusion psychosis”), emotions (“anxiety-happiness psychosis”) or  psychomotility (“hyperkinetic-akinetic motility psychosis”); and within the “unipolar-monomorph” diseases the signal difference between “pure mania/melancholia” and the “pure euphorias/depressions” is that in “pure mania” and in “pure melancholia” the entire “mental structure” is affected, whereas in the “pure euphorias” and “pure depressions” only parts of the mental structure are involved.

 Leonhard’s classification of “endogenous psychoses” was published in 1957 just about the time when neuropsychopharmacology was born.

 

Karl Leonhard

1957

 

Classification of Endogenous Psychoses

 

UNIPOLAR

Pure Mania

Pure Melancholia

Pure Euphorias

unproductive, hypochondriacal, enthusiastic, confabulatory, non-participatory

Pure Depressions

harried, hypochondriacal, self-torturing, suspicious, non-participatory

 

SystematicSchizophrenias

paraphrenias (hypochondriacal, phonemic, incoherent, fantastic, confabulatory, expansive), hebephrenias (silly, eccentric, insipid, autistic) catatonias (parakinetic, affected, proskinetic, negativistic, voluble, sluggish)

                                                             

BIPOLAR

Manic Depressive Psychosis

 

Cycloid Psychoses

excited/inhibited confusion psychosis; anxiety/happiness psychosis

hyperkinetic/akinetic motility psychosis

 

Unsystematic Schizophrenias

cataphasia, affect-laden paraphrenia, periodic catatonia

 

Forgetting the Language of Psychiatry

         The dream of Moreau de Tours’ (1845) in the mid-19th century to use drugs in the study of insanity has become a realistic goal in the mid-1950s with the introduction of effective pharmacological treatments, such as chlorpromazine, reserpine,  imipramine and iproniazid in psychiatry; the demonstration of the presence of monoamine neurotransmitters in the brain, such as norepinephrine and serotonin; the recognition of chemical mediation at the site of the synapse; and the construction of the spectrophotofluorimeter (Ban 1969; Bowman, Caulfield and Udenfriend 1955; Delay and Deniker 1952; Kline 1958; Kuhn 1957; Twarog and Page 1953; Vogt 1954). The capability to measure changes in the concentration of neurotransmitter monoamines and their metabolites in the brain led to the development of neuropharmacology, a branch of pharmacology that deals with the detection of the mode of action of centrally acting drugs. It has also opened the path for the development of neuropsychopharmacology, a new discipline that studies the relationship between neuronal and mental events with the employment of centrally acting drugs (Ban and Ucha Udabe 2006). By the end of 1955, the year the new technology (spectrophotofluorimetry) became available, Pletscher, Shore and Brodie (1955) at the National Heart Institute in the United States, reported a decrease in brain serotonin levels after the administration of reserpine, a substance that was seen to induce depression in some patients when used in the treatment to hypertension (Freis 1954; Mueller, Pryor, Gibbons and Orgain 1955). And in 1956, one year later, Pletscher (1956) first, and then Besendorf and Pletscher (1956), reported increase in brain serotonin levels after the administration of iproniazid, a monoamine oxidase inhibitor that was reported to induce euphoria in some tubercular patients in the course of treatment (Flaherty 1952; Selikoff, Robitzek and Orenstein 1952). 

         One of the first to recognize that neuropsychopharmacology opened a new perspective in the understanding and treatment of psychiatric illness was Abraham Wikler (1957), an American psychiatrist and pharmacologist. In his monograph on The Relation of Psychiatry to Pharmacology, published in 1957, he entertained the possibility that studying the mode of action of psychotropic drugs with known therapeutic effects might lead to the neurochemical underpinning of mental disorders, a pre-requisite for the development of rational treatments.

         In the 1960s a paradigm shift in psychiatry from psychopathology/psychodynamics to psychopharmacology followed, and by the 1970s with hopes that using drugs would get directly to the biological substrate of mental pathology, interest from psychopathology and psychiatric nosology turned to research in the biochemistry of psychiatric disease.

         Neuropsychopharmacology received wings in the 1960s from Arvid Carlsson’s (1961) report on selective changes on brain monoamines with psychotropic drugs. His findings set the stage for a development that led to the formulation of the catecholamine hypothesis of affective disorders by Joseph Schildkraut in 1965, and Bunney and Davis (1965) independently, and the dopamine hypothesis of schizophrenia by Jacques Van Rossum in 1967.

         Yet, they were warning signals already in the early years that something was wrong. It was apparent to all those working with patients from the beginning that one of the essential prerequisites of neuropsychopharmacological research, a clearly identified treatment responsive population, was not fulfilled. The heterogeneity in pharmacological responsiveness to the new drugs was so great within the diagnostic groups that it took eight years (1952-60) to demonstrate the therapeutic efficacy of chlorpromazine in schizophrenia, and seven years (from 1957 to 1965) to demonstrate the therapeutic efficacy of imipramine in depression (Casey, Lasky, Klett and Hollister 1960; Delay and Deniker 1952; Klerman and Cole 1965; Kuhn 1957).  

         To overcome the difficulties created by the heterogeneity within the diagnostic groups for the demonstration of therapeutic efficacy of a rapidly growing number of new psychotropic dugs, a regulatory requirement for introducing a drug for clinical use in some countries by the early 1950s, a statistical methodology, the randomized clinical trial (RCT) was adopted. It was hoped that the data collected in RCTs would help to resolve the heterogeneity within the diagnoses by identifying treatment responsive populations with the use of linear regression equations or other statistical methods. But this was not to be the case (Roth and Barnes 1981). To meet the needs of RCTs for reliable diagnostic end-points, consensus-based diagnoses, such as the DSM-III, and for the detection and documentation of changes, sensitized rating scales were adopted (American Psychiatric Association 1980; 1987, 1994; Guy 1976). Since consensus-based diagnoses cover up their component diagnoses and rating scales are sensitized by retaining only the most sensitive symptoms and signs to treatment, their use has precluded the possibility of studying “psychopathology” and indices relevant to “psychiatric nosology,” in the collected data in the numerous clinical studies, to find relevant information about the treatment responsive subpopulations within the diagnostic groups (Ban 2006).  

         During the 1960s and ‘70s there was still hope that the pharmacological heterogeneity within the diagnostic group will be resolved by the replacement of old psychopathology-based diagnoses with diagnoses built from new buildings blocks based on biological measures, such as neuroendocrine tests, biochemical changes, neurophysiological indicators and/or brain images  (Buchsbaum and Haier 1978; Carroll 1985; Dreger 1968). It was only in the 1980s, after the introduction of DSM-III, that it became evident that this was not to be the case.

         By 1987, the time of the postulation of a “clinical prerequisite” for rendering findings in neuropsychopharmacological and biological research in psychiatry interpretable, psychopathology and psychiatric nosology became forgotten languages in psychiatry {3} (Ban 1987).

         Itwas also forgotten that in the late 1950s Christian Astrup (1959), a Norwegian professor of psychiatry, and in the mid-1960s Frank Fish (1964), a British professor of psychiatry had shown that pharmacological heterogeneity in schizophrenia could be considerably reduced by adopting Karl Leonhard’s (1957, 1979) classification of endogenous psychoses.

         The findings of Fish were especially convincing. By re-classifying  474 schizophrenic patents with the employment of Leonhard’s diagnostic criteria, he found moderate to marked response to neuroleptics in as many as 79% of his 123 patients with the diagnosis of “non-systematic (unsystematic) schizophrenia” and only in 23% of his 351 patients with the diagnosis of “systematic schizophrenia.”  In addition to the more than three-fold difference in responsiveness to neuroleptics between the two groups, Fish also revealed that from his 51 patients with “affect-laden paraphrenia,”  a form of  “non-systematic (unsystematic) schizophrenia,”  characterized by delusions  with intense emotional participation  (delusional dynamics), more than 4 in 5 patients (43 or 84.4%) had a moderate  to marked response to treatment, whereas from his 100 patients “with systematic hebephrenia,” less than 1 in 4 (23 or 23%) had a similar response.

 

Frank Fish (1964)

Schizophrenia

(474 patients)

 

Marked to Moderate Response to Phenothiazine Tranquilizers

    

UNSYSTEMATIC SCHIZOPHRENIAS   SYSTEMATIC SCHIZOPHRENIAS

79% of 123                                                 23% of 351

Affect-laden Paraphrenia                        Systematic Hebephrenias

84.4% from 51                                              23% of 100

More than 4 in 5                                          Less than 1 in 4

 

Rediscovering the Language of Psychiatry

 

Revival of Nosology

 

Diagnostic Criteria for Research

         In spite of Fish’s (1964) report, Leonhard’s (1957) classification was dormant for almost 20 years. Then, in 1982, a guide to Leonhard’s classification of chronic schizophrenias (GUIDE) was introduced at the Tennessee Neuropsychiatric Institute (TNI) of Vanderbilt University (Nashville); and two years later, in 1984, a Hungarian team, led by Bertalan Pethö published research diagnostic criteria with the title, KDK Budapest for use in diagnosing functional psychoses (in Hungarian) that was based primarily on Leonhard’s work (Ban 1982; Pethö, Ban, Kelemen, et al. 1984).

         In the mid-1980s the KDK Budapest was adopted with some modifications from Hungarian into English at the TNI with the title DCR Budapest-Nashville in the Diagnosis and Classification of Functional Psychoses. The DCR was published in 1988 by Pethö and Ban in collaboration with András Kelemen, Gabor Ungvari, István Karczag, István Bittér, and Judith Tolna from Budapest, and Marek Jarema (from Poland), François Ferrero (from Switzerland), Eugenio Aguglia (from Italy), Giovanni Luca Zurria (from Italy) and Olaf Fjetland (from the United states) at the time working with Thomas Ban at Vanderbilt in  Nashville. {4}

         Neither the KDK Budapest, nor the DCR Budapest-Nashville, is restricted to Leonhard’s (1957) diagnostic concepts of “endogenous psychoses.” Both include also Wimmer’s (1916) diagnostic concept of “psychogenic (reactive) psychoses” and the German diagnostic concept of “delusional development” formulated by Robert Gaupp (1914) and Ernst Kretschmer (1927), at the University of Tübingen (Faergeman 1945; Perris 1973; Retterstol 1978, Strömgren 1974).

         At the core of the DCR is a diagnostic decision tree that consists of 524 variables, organized into 179 diagnostic decision clusters that yield 21 tentative, 33 provisional, 45 working and 55 final diagnoses. The variables of the DCR are almost exclusively psychopathological symptoms, as in the course of KDK development it was found that adding variables of social adjustment lowered predictive validity of the diagnoses (Pethö 1984; Pethö, Tolna and Tusnády 1979; Strauss and Carpenter 1974).        

         Findings with the GUIDE revealed that the significantly different response to neuroleptics in the two classes of schizophrenias is not restricted to therapeutic effects but extend to adverse reactions. Analyses of data of an international survey conducted with 768 chronic schizophrenic patients showed that tardive dyskinesia (TD) occurred more than three times more frequently in patients diagnosed with “systematic schizophrenia” (13.3%) than in patients diagnosed with “non-systematic (unsystematic)  schizophrenia” (4.3%) (Guy, Ban and Wilson 1985). Since in Fish’s study moderate to marked response to neuroleptics was more than three times as frequent in the “non-systematic schizophrenias” (79%) than in the “systematic schizophrenias” (23%), the inverse relationship between therapeutic effects and TD (i.e., less TD if the drug works and more TD if it does not) indicates that the functional state of the structures involved in the mode of action of neuroleptics is different in the “systematic schizophrenias” from the “non-systematic schizophrenias.”

 

Guy, Ban and Wilson (1985)
International Survey

768 Chronic Schizophrenic Patients

TARDIVE DYSKINESIA

Present

UNSYSTEMATIC SCHIZOPHRENIAS      SYSTEMATIC SCHIZOPHRENIAS

4.3%                                                         13.3%

(Fish: 79% response rate)                 (Fish: 23% response rate)

 

         The functional state of the structures involved in the mode of action of lithium is also different in the “systematic schizophrenias” from the “non-systematic schizophrenias.” In a survey conducted in 24 schizophrenic patients whose neuroleptic medication was supplemented with lithium to potentiate therapeutic effects it was found that 9 of the 10 patients from the population with “non-systematic schizophrenia” responded favourably to the lithium supplementation, whereas 9 of the 14 of patients diagnosed with “systematic schizophrenia” responded unfavourably. It was also noted that 5 of the 14 patients diagnosed with “systematic schizophrenia” developed neurotoxicity to lithium supplementation whereas in the population with “non-systematic schizophrenia” neurotoxicity was not encountered at all (Prakash, Kelwala and Ban 1982). 

           

COMPOSITE DIAGNOSTIC EVALUATION

         While the DCR was still in development the DSM-III of the American Psychiatric Association (1980), introduced in 1980, became an unprecedented success. By combining both major traditions of medicine, the tradition of Galen (131-201 AD), focussed on disease (Axis 1 and Axis III), and the tradition of Hippocrates (460-370 BC), focused on patient (Axis IV and Axis V), the DSM-III with its multi-axial diagnoses was received with open arms by practicing psychiatrists (Ban 2000). Yet, the adoption of DSM-III was counterproductive for neuropsychopharmacological research. Progress in neuropsychopharmacology is dependent on pharmacologically homogeneous populations, as indicated before, and the DSM-III reified pharmacologically heterogeneous diagnostic concepts which, for psychopathologists were only “ideas” and not carved-in-stone realities. It also blocked nosological research in psychiatry that “guided by the idea of disease entity” gave preference to certain, particular elements of the clinical picture to “isolate for diagnostic purposes relative disease-entities” that best fit the needs of a  particular research (Jaspers 1913).  

         To provide a methodology for uncovering diagnostic concepts that might fit better the needs of neuropsychopharmacological research than consensus-based diagnoses, a Composite Diagnostic Evaluation (CODE) System was developed in the late 1980s and early 1990s (Ban 1989).

         The CODE System is a set of instruments that can assign to a patient simultaneously a diagnosis from several diagnostic systems. Each instrument (CODE) can provide for a poly-diagnostic evaluation in a distinct category of mental illness by the employment of an integrated criteria list and standardized data collection; and each instrument consists of a set of symptoms (“codes”) that yield diagnoses in all its  component diagnostic systems, a semi-structured interview, suitable for the elicitation of all the symptoms (“codes”) encountered in the system, and diagnostic decision trees that organize symptoms into distinct psychiatric disorders (Ban 1991).

         The prototype of the CODE System is CODE-DD, the CODE for depressive disorders that provides a depressive diagnosis in 25 different classifications (from Kraepelin’s to the DSM-III-R) of depressive disorders on the basis of the “presence” or “absence” of 90 symptoms (“codes”), determined in 30 to 40 minutes with the use of a semi-structured interview that can be administered with or without computer prompting (Ban, Fjetland, Kutcher and Morey 1993).    

         CODE-DD is a reliable and valid instrument. In the first reliability study, there was an 87.8% inter-rater agreement on the presence or absence of the 90-items of the vocabulary; in the second, inter-rater agreement increased to 100%; and in two validation studies, the correspondence between the clinical DSM-III-R diagnosis of “major depression,” and the CODE-DD diagnosis of “major depression,” was 99.6% and 97.2%, respectively (Ban, Fjetland, Kutcher and Morey 1993; Morey 1991).

         CODE-DD was translated and adopted from the English original into several languages, including Estonian by  Mehilane (1992); French by Ferrero, Crocq and Dreyfus (1992); Italian by Aguglia and Forti (1989); Polish by  Pużyński, Jarema and Wdowiak (1989); and Portuguese by Nardi  and Versiani (1990). Early development of the instrument was linked to clinical studies with reboxetine. It was used in a series of clinical trials that led to the demonstration of the therapeutic efficacy of reboxetine, a selective norepinephrine re-uptake blocker, in “major depression” (Ban, Gaszner, Aguglia et al. 1998).

         Findings with CODE-DD correspond with the commonly held view that the DSM-III-R diagnosis of major depression is a broad diagnostic category. If depressive illness were characterized by unmotivated depressed mood, depressive evaluations, and lack of reactive mood changes, as in the latest version of CODE-DD, from the 322 patients with the clinical diagnosis of major depression, included in the second validation study of CODE-DD, only 119 patients, i.e., 37%, would have qualified for depression.  Findings with CODE-DD are also in keeping with the notion that depression consists of more than one form of illness; from the 322 patients only 95 patents, i.e., 29.5%, fulfilled definite criteria of Kraepelin’s (1896, 1913) depressive states, characterized by motor retardation, retardation of thought and difficulties of concentration, and even less, 45 patients, i.e., 14%, fulfilled criteria of Schneider’s (1920) vital depression. The overlap between the two forms of depressive illness was negligible (Ban, 2001, 2007).

         Yet, however broad the DSM-III-R diagnosis of “major depression” is, it covers up diagnostic concepts with possible relevance to treatment. For example, the diagnosis of Schneider’s (1920) “vital depression” with the cardinal symptoms of corporization, disturbance of vital balance and feeling of loss of vitality, that provided the key for  Roland Kuhn (1957) to recognize imipramine’s antidepressant effect, is covered up to the extent that even if the patient is so severely ill that it displays all possible symptoms and signs for the diagnosis of “major depression” in the DSM-IV, one still would not know whether the patient qualifies for vital depression (American Psychiatric Association 1994). The same applies to Kraepelin’s (1913) depressive states.

 

FINDINGS WITH CODE-DD

         Number (and percentage) of the 322/233 patients with a DSM-III-R diagnosis of major depression fulfilling criteria of depressive illness in a selected number of classifications included in CODE-DD

 

COMPOSITE DIAGNOSTIC CLASSIFICATION

(Ban 1989)

322 patients

unmotivated depressed mood, depressive evaluations & lack of reactive mood changes

119 (37%)

 

KURT SCHNEIDER’S VITAL DEPRESSION

(Schneider 1920)

233 patients

corporization, disturbance of vital balance, and feeling of loss of vitality

45 (14%)

 

EMIL KRAEPELIN’S DEPRESSIVE STATES

(Kraepelin 1913)

233 patients

depressed mood, motor retardation, thought retardation

95 (28.5%)

 

REVIVAL OF PSYCHOPATHOLOGY

         During the last decade of the 20th century, molecular genetics has entered the psychiatric scene and by the dawn of the 21st century genes encoding the primary targets of psychotropic drugs in the brain, such as G-protein-coupled receptors, nuclear-hormonal receptors, ion channels, enzymes, etc., were identified. It was also recognised that any empirically derived treatment responsive population to a psychotropic drug could serve as a reference point for testable genetic hypotheses about mental illness with the employment of a candidate gene approach. Although patients were still diagnosed with consensus-based diagnoses at the clinic exploration of the molecular genetic basis of the biochemistry of the different diagnostic populations began (Lerer 2002).

         To meet the new needs, in 2002 a new methodology for the identification of clinical populations for research was proposed: “nosologic homotyping” with pharmacological validation (Ban 2002). 

 

NOSOLOGIC HOMOTYPING

         Nosologic homotyping is based on “structural psychopathology,” a term coined by Gyula Nyirö (1958), professor of psychiatry at the Medical University of Budapest (now Semmelweis University) in the 1950s,  for a branch of psychopathology in which Wernicke’s (1900, 1906) “mental structure” is combined with 20th century psychopathology (Jasper 1913, 1962).

         In structural psychopathology the three components (phases) of Wernicke’s  “mental structure” (psychosensory, intrapsychic and psychomotor), are extended into three “psychic structures,” the “afferent-cognitive,” the “central-affective,” and the “efferent–adaptive,” in which each hierarchical structure consists of different levels, with each level functionally connected with each other within and across structures.  In defining the functional activity of the different levels in each structure in the processing of signals in the brain, Nyirö (1962) used an ontogenetic model and suggested that the  five levels of the afferent-cognitive structure correspond with:  (1) diffuse sensation, (2) differentiated perception, (3) image formation, (4) concrete ideation and (5) abstract ideation; the four levels of the central- relational structure correspond with: (1) undifferentiated primitive (“ancient”) signal, (2) sensorial and vital emotions, (3) intellectual emotions and (4) ethical, moral and social emotions; and, the six levels of the efferent-adaptive structure correspond with (1) autonomic (“vegetative”) movements and simple (elementary) reflexes, (2) uncoordinated movements, (3) emotional and instinctual stereotypes, (4) echo phenomena, (5) voluntary goal directed coordinated movements and (6) automatisms.    

 

Gyula Nyirö (1958, 1962)

 

Structural Psychopathology

 

STRUCTURES

 

Ontogenetic Model

 

afferent-cognitive                   central-affective            efferent-adaptive

6. automatisms         

5. abstract ideation                ethical, social emotions voluntary movements

4. concrete ideation               intellectual emotions     echo phenomena

3. image formation                vital emotion                 emotional stereotypes

2. differentiated perception   sensorial emotions         uncoordinated movements

1. diffuse sensation                undifferentiated signal   simple reflexes       

 

         Within the frame of reference of structural psychopathology, psychopathological symptoms arise from abnormalities in processing of signals within and across different levels in these structures, and the nature of the abnormality corresponds with the site of the abnormality in processing, e.g., an abnormality of processing from “concrete ideation” to “abstract ideation” may yield “concretization” or from “abstract ideation” to “ethical, social emotions” to “constricted affect.” 

         Nosologic homotypes are identical in “elementary units” (psychopathological symptoms), i.e., processing of signals in neuronal circuits, but to qualify for a “nosologic homotype,” abnormality in the processing of signals does not suffice.

         Nosologic homotyping is based on the assumption that even if symptoms follow their disease, as already noted by Galen in the 2nd century, it is the disease that defines its symptoms. Hence, to qualify for a “nosologic homotype”, a “psychopathology-based homotype” has to be assigned to the same position in the “nosologic matrix,” constructed with consideration of the three classifying principles of psychiatric nosology, i.e., Cullen’s (1772) “totality,” (i.e., “universal” or “partial”), Kraepelin’s (1896) “temporality” (i.e.,   “continuous” or “episodic”), and Leonhard’s (1957) “polarity” (i.e., ‘bipolar” or  “unipolar”) (Ban, 2000, 2002).

         Each pharmacologically valid distinct “nosologic homotype” provides a potential diagnostic concept for a mental disorder in which psychopathologic symptom represent distinct abnormalities in the processing of signals between levels within and across three “mental structures,”  the formal characteristics of the “onset” (sudden or insidious), “course“ (episodic or continuous) and “outcome (recovery or defect) of the mental syndrome reflect the pathological process in its “dynamic totality” and the “dynamic totality” of the pathological process, as a whole  provides a structure that is determined by the illness (Ban 1987, 1992, 2002, 2007; Pethö 1990).  

         Pharmacologically validated “nosologic homotypes” are more homogeneous populations in terms of psychopathology and psychiatric nosology than populations identified by any other method. Since a treatment responsive population is a prerequisite for neuropsychopharmacological research and for discriminate use of psychotropic drugs, “nosologic homotypes” are today the closest fit for neuropsychopharmacological and molecular genetic research, as well as for research in the pharmacotherapy of psychiatric disorders.

         Considering that in “nosologic homotypes,” psychopathological symptoms are perceived as abnormalities in the processing of signals in neuronal circuits of the human brain that is dominated by CRs, “nosologic homotypes” might provide the missing link for using abnormalities of CR parameters for bridging “psychopathology” with “pathophysiology” in the central nervous system (CNS).

 

THE CONDITIONED REFLEX REVISITED

         It was also Nyirö first to conceptualize structural psychopathology within the frame of reference of conditioning. In his essay on “The Structural aspect of mental processes on the basis of reflex mechanisms”, published in 1957, he suggested that in the formation of mental structures, “differential inhibition” and in the regulation of connections between mental structures “retarded inhibition” plays a prominent role.  Both “differential inhibition” and retarded inhibition” are manifestations of ”internal inhibition” in Pavlov’s “brain model.”

         In keeping with Nyirö’s conceptualization were Astrup’s (1962) findings in the early 1960s which indicated abnormalities of “internal inhibition,” as measured by CR differentiation and CR retardation, in chronic schizophrenia.

         A great impetus for studying CR variables in clinical research in psychiatry was given by findings in electrophysiological studies in the late 1950s which indicated that in the formation of CRs brain stem and mid-brain reticular nuclei, as well as intra-laminar nuclei of the thalamus are involved (Ban 1964; Gastaut 1958).

         To render CR variables accessible to research a “conditioning test procedure” was developed in the early 1960s in the Department of Psychiatry at McGill University  (Montreal) with the employment first only the eyelid closure technique  (Ban and Levy 1964). The eight CR parameters studied with the employment of the procedure were: (1) the extinction of the orienting reflex (OR extinction), (2) CR acquisition, (3) CR extinction, (4) CR generalization, (5) CR differentiation, (6) CR delay, (7) secondary CR formation and (8) CR reversal. The procedure was used in studying CR functions in schizophrenia and depression, as well as in clinical investigations with psychotropic drugs. Findings in these studies were published in several reports, including a monograph, Experimental Approaches to Psychiatric Diagnoses (Ban and Kerenyi 1964; Ban and Lehmann 1971; Ban, Lehmann and Green 1969, 1970; Choi, Ban, Lehmann and Adamo 1966; Hattangadi, Lidsky and Ban 1966; Hattangadi, Lidsky, Lee and Ban 1968).

         In the late 1960s the conditioning-test procedure at McGill was replaced by a conditioning test battery with the employment of seven conditioning techniques for the study of psychopathological mechanisms and psychopharmacological effects (Ban, Lehmann and Saxena 1970). The battery included (1) galvanic skin reflex (GSR), (2) salivary secretion, (3) eyelid closure, (4) defensive finger withdrawal, (5) Ivanov-Smolensky’s test for second signal system activity (1954), (6) a modification of Astrup’s (1962) word association test and (7) a modification of Lehmann’s (1968) active avoidance procedure. Yet, by 1970, the time the “battery” became fully operational, behavioral pharmacology was replaced by neuropharmacology and interest in clinical research with the behavioural conditioning method was lost.

         While clinical research in conditioning was abandoned, basic research in conditioning continued and by the dawn of the 21st century Joseph Knoll (2005), a Hungarian professor of pharmacology, recognized that the human cerebral cortex with its 10 billion neurons and its 1 million billion connections has the capacity to accommodate the steadily growing number of new CR connections throughout life (Edelman 1992).  Eric Kandel (2007, 2009), a  Nobel Laureate, found that while the architecture of behaviour, the neural circuits of the brain, remain constant, i.e., the same cells invariably hook up with the same cells, the strength of synaptic connections is getting stronger with “learning” and getting weaker  with “habituation.”   Kandel with his associates had also shown the neuronal circuits of classical conditioning (withdrawal reflex) in Aplysia Californica (Carew, Walters and Kandel 1981).

         Learning and conditioning entered molecular genetic research with Holger Hyden’s (1970) recognition in the 1960s that only part of the genome, about 5% to10%, is active at birth and the rest of gene areas activated by external factors. Hyden, another Nobel Laureate, had also shown that external factors, e.g., sensory stimulation, give rise to increased synthesis of messenger ribonucleic acid (mRNA), a  prerequisite for the “activation of hitherto silent brain areas” when learning (conditioning) is involved. Furthermore, as early as in the 1980s the possibility was entertained that even if  CR formation, the opening up of new, formerly non-operating paths in the brain are under genetic control, activation of these paths are dependent on external factors (Ban and Guy 1985). Nevertheless, in spite of all progress in discovering the biology of learning, the nagging question about the relationship between mental pathology and CR functions, has remained unanswered to-date.

 

FROM PSYCHIATRY TO NEURONOLOGY

         During the past 200 years the language of psychiatry has been continuously changing reflecting the changes in the conceptualization of insanity.

         The term “psychic reflex,” introduced by Griesinger in 1843  was replaced by the term  “conditioned reflex” by Pavlov (1906) in the early years of the 20th century; the term “neurosis,”  introduced  by Cullen in 1777 was dismissed with the publication of DSM-III in 1980; the use of the term, “psychosis,” adopted by Feuchtersleben in 1845  was narrowed by Kurt Schneider (1950)  in the   middle of the 20th century by restricting its use to  “psychiatric disease” distinct from personality anomalies; and the term, “psychiatry,” coined by Reil in 1908, with its  implicit separation of the mind (“psyche”) from the “body,” became anachronistic with the birth of neuropsychopharmacology in the 1950s.

         Considering that within our current frame of reference “psychic activity” is based on processing of signals in neuronal circuits in the brain, with some of the neuronal passes already mapped by Falck-Hillarps’ histochemical fluorescence method, a term like “neuronology” would correspond more closely with a contemporary concept of “insanity,” than “psychiatry” (Dahlström and Fuxe 1964; Falck, Hillarp, Thieme and Torp 1962; Fuxe and Dahlström 1964).  

         While the search for a language that would help translate insanity into biology continues and the conceptualization of insanity keeps on changing Porter’s (2002) contention in the early years of the 21st century, that “Madness may be as old as mankind,” has remained just as true today as Charcot’s (1877) contention that “Disease is from of old there has always been and nothing about it changes; it is we who change, as we learn to recognize what was formerly imperceptible,” was about 150 years ago. 

 

References:

Alzheimer A. Über eine Eigenartige Erkrankung der Hirnrinde. Allg Z Psychiat 1907; 64: 146-8.

American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Third Edition. (DSM – III). Washington: American Psychiatric Association; 1980.

American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Third Edition Revised (DSM-III-R). Washington: American Psychiatric Association; 1987.

American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. Fourth Edition (DSM-IV). Washington: American Psychiatric Association; 1994.

Ananth JV, Ban TA, Lehmann HE. Test-retest performance in chronic schizophrenia on the Verdun Conditioning Program. Can Psychiat Assoc J 1972; 1 (special supplement): 227-39.

Astrup C. The effects of ataraxic drugs on schizophrenia subgroups related to experimental findings. Acta psychiatr Scand 1959; 34 (supplement 136): 88-93.   

Astrup C. Conditional Reflex Studies in Schizophrenia. Springfield: Charles C. Thomas; 1962.

Ban TA. Conditioning and Psychiatry. Chicago: Aldine 1964; London: George Allen & Unwin; 1966.  

Ban TA. Psychopharmacology.  Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins; 1969.

Ban TA. Chronic schizophrenias: a guide to Leonhard’s classification. Comprehensive Psychiatry 1982; 23: 155-70.

Ban TA. Academic psychiatry and the pharmaceutical industry. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology & Biological Psychiatry 2006; 30: 429-41.

Ban TA, Guy W.. Conditioning and learning in relation to disease. Activ nerv sup (Prague) 1985; 27: 236 - 44.

Ban TA. Prolegomenon to the clinical prerequisite: psychopharmacology and the classification of mental disorders. Progress in Neuro-Psychopharmacology and Biological Psychiatry 1987; 1: 527 - 80.

Ban TA. Composite Diagnostic Evaluation of Depressive Disorders. CODE-DD. Brentwood: JM Productions; 1989.

Ban TA.  CODE-DD Valutazione e Diagnosi Dei Disturbi Depressivi. (Edited and translated from the English original into Italian by Aguglia E and Forti B). Padua:  Liviana; 1989.

Ban TA. CODE-DD Zlożona ocean diagnostyczna. (Edited and translated from the English original into Polish by Pużyński S, Jarema M, Wdowiak J). Warsaw: Prasowe Zaklady Graficzne w Koszlinie; 1989.  

Ban TA. CODE-DD Avalicão Diagnostica Composta Dos Distúrbios Depressivos. (Edited and translated from the English original into Portuguese by Nardi AE and Versiani M). Rio de Janiero: Instituto de Psiquiatria-Universidad Federal; 1990.

Ban TA. CODE Systems: Theory and practice. In: Aguglia E, Ban TA, editors. Functional Psychoses Today. Rome: John Libey; 1991. 

Ban TA. CODE-DD Evaluation diagnostique composite des troubles depressifs. (Edited and translated from the English original into French by Ferrero F, Crocq M-A, Dreyfus F).  Geneve: Editions Medicine et Hygiene; 1992.

Ban TA. CODE DD Depressiivsete haäirete diagnostika koondsüsteem. (Edited and translated from the English original into Estonian by Mehilane L.)  Tartu: Tõlge eesti keeled; 1992.

Ban TA. From DSM-III to DSM-IV: progress or standstill. In: Franzek E, Ungvari GS, Ruther E, Beckmann H, editors. Progress in Differentiated Psychopathology. Wurzburg: International Wernicke-Kleist-Leonhard Society; 2000.

Ban TA. Nosology in the teaching of psychiatry. Jornal Brasileiro de Psquiatria 2000; 49: 39-49.

Ban TA. Neuropsychopharmacology the interface between genes and neuropsychopharmacology. In: Lerer B, editor. Pharmacogenetics of Psychotropic Drugs. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2002, pp. 36-56.

Ban TA. Towards a clinical methodology for neuropsychopharmacology research. Neuropsychopharmcologia Hungarica 2007; 9: 81-90.

Ban TA, Fjetland OK, Kutscher M, Morey LC. CODE-DD Development of a diagnostic scale for depressive disorders. Hindmarch L, Stonier PD, editors. Human Psychopharmacology. Measures and Methods. Volume 4. Chichester: John Wiley & Sons; 1993, pp. 73-85.

Ban TA, Gaszner P, Aguglia E, Batista R, Castillo A, Lipcsey A, Macher J-P, Torres-Ruis A, Vergara L. Clinical efficacy of reboxetine: a comparative study with desipramine with methodological considerations. Human Psychopharmacology 1998; 13 (supplement 1):   l29-39.

Ban TA, Kerenyi N. Some aspects of conditioning and its morphological basis. Journal of Neuropsychiatry 1964; 5:  206-9. 

Ban TA, Lehmann HE. Experimental Approaches to Psychiatric Diagnosis. Springfield: Charles C. Thomas; 1971.

Ban TA, Lehmann HE, Green AA. Conditioning in the prediction of therapeutic outcome in depressions. Conditional Reflex 1969; 4: 115-23.

Ban TA, Lehmaan HE, Green AA. Conditional reflex variables in the prediction of therapeutic responsiveness to phenothiazines in the schizophrenias. In: Wittenborn JR, Goldberg SC, May RPA, editors. Psychopharmacology and the Individual Patient. New York: Raven Press; 1970.

Ban TA, Lehmann HE, Saxena B. A conditioning test battery for the study of psychopathological mechanisms and psychopharmacological effects.  Canad Psychiat Assoc J 1970; 1970; 15: 1 -+ 8.

Ban TA, Levy L. Physiological patterns: A diagnostic test procedure based on the conditioned reflex method.   Journal of Neuropsychiatry 1961; 2: 228-31.

Ban TA, Ucha Udabe. The neurotransmitter era in neuropsychopharmacology. In Ban TA, Ucha Udabe R, editors. The Neurotransmitter Era in Neuropsychopharmacology. Buenos Aires: Polemos; 2006, pp. 265-74

Bell Ch. Idea of a New Anatomy of the Brain. London; Strahan 1811.

Berner P, Gabriel E, Katschnig  H, Keffer W, Koehler, Lenz G, Simhandl Ch. Diagnostic Criteria  for Schizophrenia and Affective Psychoses. Washington: World Psychiatric Association; 1983.

Besendorf H, Pletscher A. Beeinflüssung zentraler Wirkungen mit Reserpin und 5-Hydroxytryptamin durch isonicotinesauerehydrazide.  Helv Physol Acta 1956; 14: 383-90.  

Bleuler E. Dementia Praecox oder Gruppe der Schizophrenien. Leipzig: Deuticke; 1911. 

Bowman RL, Caulfield PA, Udenfriend S. Spectrophotometric assay in the visible and ultraviolet. Science 1955; 122:  32-3.

Buchsbaum MS, Haier RJ. Biological homogeneity, symptom heterogeneity, and the diagnosis of schizophrenia. Schizophrenia Bulletin 1978; 4: 473-5.

Bunney WE, Davis JM. Norepinephrine in depressive reaction. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1965; 3: 483-94.    

Bykov  KM. The Cerebral Cortex and the Internal Organs. (Translated from the original Russian into English by Gantt WH). Chicago:  Chemical Publishers Company; 1957.

Cajal SR. La fine structure des centres nerveux.  Proc R Soc London 1894; 55: 444-67.

Carew TJ, Walters ET, Kandel ER. Classical conditioning in a simple withdrawal reflex in Aplysia Californica. Journal of Neuroscience 1981; 1 1426 -37.

Carlsson A. Brain monoamines and psychotropic drugs. In: Rothlin E, editor. Neuropsychopharmacology. Amsterdam: Elsevier; 1961.

Carroll BJ. Dexamethasone suppression test: A review of contemporary confusion. J Clin Psychiatry 1985; 46: 13-24.

Casey JF, Lasky JJ, Klett CJ, Hollister LE. Treatment of schizophrenic reactions with phenothiazine derivatives. Am J Psychiatry 1960; 117: 95-105.

Charcot J-M. Lectures on the diseases of the nervous system. Delivered at the Salpêtriere in Paris. (Translated from the French original into English by Sigerson G). London: New Sydenham Society; 1877.

Choi SM, Ban TA, Lehmann HE, Adamo E. Conditional reflex studies on the effect of psychoactive drugs in schizophrenics. Laval medical 1966; 37: 122-5.

Cullen W. Synopsis Nosologiae Methodicae. Edinburgh: Kincaid & Creech; 1769.

Cullen W. First Lines of the Practice of Physics. Edinburgh: Kincaid & Creech; 1772.

Dahlström A, Fuxe K. Evidence for the existence of monoamine-containing neurons in the central nervous system. I. Demonstration of monoamines in the cell bodies of brain stem neurons. Acta Physiol Scan 1964; 62 (supplement 232): 1- 55. 

Delay J, Deniker P. Le traitments de psychoses par une méthode neurolytique dérivée de

l’hibernothérapie; le 4560 RP utilisée seul en cure prolongée et continue. CR Congr Méd Alién Neurol (France) 1952; 50: 497-502.

Dreger RM. Aristotle, Linnaeus and Lewin, or the place of classification in the therapeutic-evaluative process. J gen Psychol 1968; 78: 41-59.

Edelman GM. Bright Air, Brilliant Fire. New York: Basic Books; 1992.

Esquirol JED. Des maladies mentales Considerees sous les raports medical, hygienique  et medico-legal. Paris: JP Bailliere; 1838.

Faergeman, PM, De psykogene psykoser belyst gennem katamnesticke undersogelser. Copenhagen: Munksgaard; 1945.

Falck B, Hillarp N-A, Thieme G, Torp A. Fluorescence of catecholamines and related compounds condensed with formaldehyde. J Histochem Cytochem 1962; 10: 34e8-54. 

Falret JP. Leçonc clinique de le mêdecine mentale faites a l’hospice de Salpêtriêre. Paris: Bailliere; 1854.

Feuchtersleben E. Lehrbuch der Ärztlichen Selenkunde. Vienna: Carl Gerold; 1845.

Fish F. Guide to Leonhard’s classification of chronic schizophrenias. Psychiatric Quarterly 1964; 38: 438-50.

Fish F. The influence of the tranquilizers on the Leonhard schizophrenic syndromes. Encéphale 1964; 53: 245-9. 

Fish F. Clinical Psychopathology. Bristol: John Wright & Sons; 1967.

Flaherty JA. The psychiatric use of isonicotinic acid hydrazide: a case report, Delaware med J 1952; 24: 298-300.

Franzek E. Influence of Carl Wernicke on Karl Leonhard’s nosology. Psychopathology 1990; 23: 277-81.

Freis D. Mental depression in hypertensive patients treated for a long period with large doses of reserpine. New England Journal of Medicine 1954; 251: 1006-8.

Fuxe K. Evidence for the existence of monoamine nerve terminals in the central nervous system.  Acta Physiol Scand 1965; 64 (supplement 247): 9-85. 

Gantt WH. Physiological psychology. Annual Review of Physiology 1948 10: 453-78.

Garrison FH. An Introduction to the History of Medicine. Philadelphia/London: WB Saunders; 1929.

Gastaut H. Some aspects of the neurophysiological basis of conditioned reflexes and behaviour. In: O’Connor, editor. Ciba Foundation Symposium on the Neurological Basis of Behavior. London: Churchill; 1958.

Gaupp R. Die wissentschaftliche Bedeutung  des “Falles Wagner”. Munich med Wschr 1914; 61:  633 – 7.

Golgi C. Sulla fine struttura dei bulbi olfattorii. Riv Sper  Freniatr Med Leg Alienazioni Ment 1874; 1: 405-25.

Griesinger W. Über psychische Reflexactionen. Archiv für Physiologische Heilkunde  1843;  2: 76-112.

Griesinger W. Die Pathologie und Therapie der PsychischenKrankheiten. Braunschweig: Wreden; 1845.

Guy W. ECDEU Assessment Manual for Psychopharmacology. Revised. Washington: US Department of Health, Education and Welfare. DHEW Publication No. (ADM)76-338; 1976. 

Guy W, Ban TA, edited and translated. The AMDP System. Manual for the Assessment and Documentation of Psychopathology. Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer; 1982.

Guy W, Ban TA, Wilson WH. An international survey of tardive dyskinesia. Prog Neuro-psychopharmacol & Biol Psychiatry 1985; 9: 401-5.

Hamilton M, editor. Fish’s Clinical Psychopathology. Bristol: John Wright & Sons; 1985.

Hattangadi S, Lidsky A, Ban TA. The orienting response (GSR) in psychiatric patients. Conditional Reflex 1966; 1: 214-5.

Hattangadi S, Lidsky A, Lee H, Ban TA. Orienting reflex behaviour and clinical psychopathology 1968; 3: 29-33.

Heinroth JC. Lehrbuch der Störungen des Seelenlebens. Leipzig: Vogel; 1818.

Hyden H. The question of the molecular basis of memory. In: Broadbent DE, editor. Biology of Memory. New York: Academic Press; 1970.

Ivanov-Smolensky AG. Essays on the Pathophysiology of Higher Nervous Activity According to IP Pavlov and his School. Moscow: Foreign Language Publishing House; 1954.

James R. Medical Dictionary. London: T. Osborne; 1743.

Jaspers K. Eifersuchtswahn: Entwicklung einer Persoenlichkeit oder Prozess. Zeitschrift für die gesamte Neurologie und Psychiatrie 1910; 1: 567-637.

Jaspers K. Allgemeine Psychopathologie. Berlin/Heidelberg: Springer; 1913.

Jaspers K. General Psychopathology. (Translated from the 7th edition of the German original into English by Hoenig J and Hamilton MW).  Manchester: Manchester University Press; 1962.  

Kahlbaum KL. Die Grouppierung der psychische Krankheiten und die Enteilung der Seelenstoerungen. Danzig: AW Kaufman; 1863.

Kahlbaum KL. Die Katatonie oder des Spannungsirresein  Berlin: Hirschfeld; 1874.  

Kandel ER. In Search of Memory. The Emergence of a New Science of Mind. New York: WW Norton; 2007.

Kandel ER. The biology of memory. A forty years perspective. J Neurosci 2009; 29: 12748-56.   

Kendell RE. Reflections on psychiatric classifications – for the architects of DSM-IV and ICD-10. Integrative Psychiatry 1984; 2: 42-7.

Kleist K. Autochtonous Degenerationpsychosen. Z gen Neurol Psychiat 1921; 69: 1-11.

Kleist K. Die Auffassung der Schizophrenien als Systemkrankheiten. Klinische Wochenschrift 1923; 2: 962-3.

Kleist K. Über zykloide, paranoid und epileptoide psychosen und über die Frage  der Degenerationspsychosen. Schweiz Arch Neurol Psychiat 1928; 13: 1-35.

Klerman GJ, Cole JO.  Clinical pharmacology of imipramine and related compounds. Pharmacol Rev 1965; 17:  101-41.  

Kline NS. Clinical experience with iproniazid (Marsilid). J Clin Exp Psychopathol 1958; 19 (supplement 1): 72-8.

Knoll J. The Brain and Its Self. A Neurochemical Concept of the Innate and Acquired Drives.  Berlin: Springer; 2005.

Kraepelin E. Lehrbuch der Psychiatrie. 4 Aufl. Barth: Leipzig; 1893.

Kraepelin E. Lehrbuch der Psychiatrie. 5 Aufl. Barth: Leipzig; 1896.

Kraepelin E. Lehrbuch der Psychiatrie. 6 Aufl. Barth: Leipzig; 1899.

Kraepelin E. Lehrbuch der Psychiatrie. 8 Aufl. Barth: Leipzig; 1913.

Kretschmer E.  Der sensitive Beziehungswahn. Berlin: Springer; 1927.

Kuhn R. Über die Behandlung depressives Zustände mit einem iminodibenzyl-derivat (G22355). Schweiz Med Wochenschr 1957; 87: 1135-40.

Lasègue EC. Du delire de persecution. Arch gen Med 1852; 28: 129-50.

Lasègue EC. Mental vertigo. Gazette des hôpitoux (Paris). 1877. In Shorter E. A Historical Dictionary of Psychiatry. Oxford: Oxford Univerity Press; 2005, p. 29.

Lehmann HE. The influence of neuroleptics and anxiolytic sedatives on conflict avoidance behaviour in human subjects. In: Volume Dedicated to the 60th Birthday of Pofessor Eugen Vencovsky. Prague: State Publishing House; 1968.   

Leonhard K. Die Defektschizophrenen Krankheitsbilder. Leipzig: Thieme; 1936.

Leonhard K. Aufteilung der endogenen Psychosen. Berlin:  Akademie Verlag; 1957.

Leonhard K. Classification of Endogenous Psychoses (translated from the 5th edition of the German original by Berman R.) New York: Irwington Press; 1979.

Lerer B. Genes and neuropsychopharmacology exploring the interface. In: Lerer B, editor. Pharmacogenetics of Psychotropic Drug. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 2002, pp. 3-17.

Littre E. Dictionnaire de la Langua Francoise. Paris; Hachette Cie; 1877.

Magendie F. Expériences  sur les  fonctions des racines des nerfs rachidiens. Journal de physiologie expérimentale et de pathologie 1822; 2: 276-9.

Menninger K. Mayman M, Pruyser P. The Vital Balance. The Life Process in Mental Health and Illness. New York: The Viking Press; 1968.

Möbius JP. Abriss der Lehre von den Nervenkrankheiten. Leipzig: Barth; 1893.

Möbius JP. Ueber die Physiologischen Schwachsinn des Weibes. Halle: Marhold; 1900.

Moreau de Tours J. Du Hachich et de L’Aliénation Mentale. Etudes Psychologiques, Paris:  Fortin & Mason; 1845.

Morey L. Reliability considerations in the development of CODE-DD. In: Aguglia E, Ban TA, editors. Functional Psychoses Today. Rome: Jon Libbey; 1991, pp. 297-304.

Mueller JC, Pryor WW, Gibbons JE, Orgain ES. Depression and anxiety occurring during Rauwolfia therapy. JAMA 1955; 159: 836-9.

Neele E. Die Phasischen Psychosen. Leipzzig: J.A. Barth; 1949.

Nyirö Gy. A lelki mukodesek strukturalis szemlelete a reflexfolyamat alapjan. (Structural aspects of mental processes on the basis of reflex mechanisms). In: Gegesi Kiss P, Kardos L, Lenard F, Molnar I, editors. Psychologiai Tanulmanyok (Studies in Psychology).  Budapest: Akademiai Kiado; 1958, pp. 265-77.

Nyiro Gy. Psychiatria. Budapest: Medicina; 1962.

Pavlov IP. The scientific investigation of the psychical faculties or processes in higher animals. Lancet 1906; 84: 911-5.

Pavlov IP. Conditioned Reflexes. (Translated from the Russian original into English by Anrep GV). Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1927.

Perris C. Cycloid psychoses: historical background and nosology. Tidsskr nord  Rets med Psykiat 1973; 27: 369-78.

Pethö B. Hourglass model of the psychiatric illness. Schizophrenia Bulletin 1984; 10: 509-19.

Petho B. Development and structure of the DCR Budapest-Nashville. Psychopathology 1990;  23: 316 -30.

Pethö B,  Ban TA, Kelemen A, Ungvari G, Karczag I, Bittér I, Tolna J. KDK Budapest. Kutatasi diagnosztikus kriteriumok functionalis psychosisok korismezesehez. (Research diagnostic criteria for diagnosing functional psychoses). Ideggyogyaszati Szemle 1984; 37: 102 - 31.

Pethö B, Ban TA, in collaboration with Kelemen A, Ungvari G, Karczag I, Bittér I, Tolna J (Budapest), Jarema M, Ferrero F, Agulia E, Zurria GL, Fjetland OK. DCR Budapest-Nashville in the Diagnosis and Classification of Functional Psychoses. Psychopathology 1988; 21 (4-): 152-240.

Pethö B, Tolna J, Tusnády G. Multi-trait/multi-method assessment of predictive variables of outcome of schizophrenia spectrum disorders. A nosological evaluation. J psychiat Res 1979; 15:  163-74.      

Pichot P. A Century of Psychiatry. Paris: Roger Dacosta; 1983.

Pinel P. Nosographie Philosophique ou la Methode de l’Analyse Appliquée a la Medicine. Paris: Brosson; 1798.

Pletscher A.  Beeinflüssung des 5-Hydroxytryptamin stoffwechsels im Gehirn durch

Isonicotinsäurehydrazide.  Experientia 1956; 12: 479-80. 

Pletscher A, Shore PA, Brodie BB. Serotonin release as possible mechanism of reserpine action, Science 1955; 122: 374-5.

Porter R. Madness. A Brief History.  Oxford:  Oxford University Press; 2002.

rakash R, Kelwala S, Ban TA. Neurotoxicity with combined administration of lithium and a neuroleptic. 1982; 23: 567- 71.

Reil JC (1808). In Reil JC, Hoffbauer JC, editors. Beitrage zur Besorderung eine Kurmethode auf psychischen Wege. Halle: In der Kurtschen Buchhandlung; 1808.

Retterstol N. The Scandinavian concept of reactive psychosis, schizophreniform psychosis and schizophrenia. Psychiatria clin 1978; 11: 180-7.

Roth M, Barnes TRE. The classification of affective disorders: a synthesis of old and new concepts. Compr Psychiatry 1981; 22: 54-77.

Sauvages de la Croix Boissier F. Nosologia Methodica. Amsterdam: Frat de Tournes; 1768.

Schildkraut JJ. The catecholamine hypothesis of affective disorders. A review of supporting evidence. Am J Psychiatry 1965; 122: 509-22.   

Schneider K. Die Schichtung des emitionalen Lebens und der Aufbau der Depressions zustände. Z Ges Neurol Psychiat 1920; 59: 81-5.

Schneider K. Klinische Psychopathologie. Stuttgart: Thieme; 1950.

Sechenov IM (1863) Reflexes of the Brain. In: Selected Works. Moscow: Foreign Language Publishers; 1935.

Selikoff IJ, Robitzek EH, Orenstein GG. Treatment of pulmonary tuberculosis with hydrazine derivatives of isonicotinic acid. JAMA 1952; 150: 973-80.

Sherrington CS. The Integrative Action of the Nervous System. London: Scribner; 1906. 

Shorter E. A Historical Dictionary in Psychiatry. Oxford/New York: Oxford University Press; 2005.

Strauss JJ, Carpenter WT. The prediction of outcome in schizophrenia. II Relationship  between predictor and outcome variables. Archs gen Psychiatry 1974; 31:37-42.

Strömgren E. Psychogenic psychoses. In: Hirsch S, Shepherd M, editors. Themes and Variations in European Psychiatry. Charlottesville: University Press of Virginia; 1974. 

Taylor MA. The Psychiatric Mental Status Examination. New York: Pergamon Press; 1981.

Twarog BM, Page IH. Serotonin content of some mammalian tissues and urine and a

method for its demonstration. Am J Physiol 1953; 175: 157-61.

Van Rossum JM, The significance of dopamine-receptor blockade for the action of neuroleptic drugs. In: Brill H,  Cole JO, Deniker P,  Hippius H,  Bradley PB, editors. Neuropsychopharmacology. Proceedings of the Fifth International Congress of the Collegium Internationale Neuro-Psychopharmacologicum (March 1966). Amsterdam:  Excerpta Medica Foundation; 1967, pp. 321-9.

Vogt M. Concentration of sympathin in different parts of central nervous system under

normal conditions and after administration of drugs. J Physiol 1954; 123: 451-81.

Walters ET, Karen TJ, Kandel E. Classical conditioning in a simple withdrawal reflex in Aplysia. California Journal of Neuroscience 1981; 1: 1426-37.  

Wells HK. Ivan Pavlov. Toward a Scientific Psychology & Psychiatry. New York International Publishers; 1956.

Wernicke C. Lehrbuch der Geisteskrankheiten für Aerzte und Studierende. Vol. 2. Berlin: Theodor Fischer Kassel; 1881 – 1883, pp  229-42.

Wernicke C. Über die Klassifikation der Psychosen. Breslau: Schletter; 1899.

Wernicke C. Grundrisse der Psyhiatrie. Leipzig: Thieme; 1900, 1906.  

Westphal C. Ueber Zwangsvorstellungen Archive fuer Psychiatrie und Nervenkrankheiten 1878; 8: 734-50.

Wikler A. The Relation of Psychiatry to Pharmacology. Baltimore: Williams & Wilkins;

1957.

Wimmer  A. Psykogene Sindssydomsformer. Copenhagen:  St. Hans Hospital 1816 – 1916 Jubilee Publication; 1916.

 

November 14, 2013

 

Barry Blackwell’s Comments

        I have read Tom Ban's brief e-book on the early evolution of neuroscience and, as always, am amazed by the breadth and depth of its erudition. The historical dissection of the evolution of psychiatry is masterful, reminding the reader (painfully?) of how much we have forgotten (or never knew) and how relevant that is to today's dead end in new drug development for specific disorders.

         I particularly liked the way it summarized each step along the way with clear, succinct statements listed in bold type. Secondly this is such a precise, compact and brief history of our antecedents and steps leading up to the contemporary impasse it should be mandatory introductory material for every person entering the mental health field at the graduate level - whether psychiatry, psychology or neuroscience. I do differ on the place of lithium which is not listed among the early synthetic compounds found serendipitously to treat mental disorders -  Cade's early clinical work was historically the first to scientifically establish the efficacy of a specific pharmacologic agent in the treatment of a specific psychiatric disorder (Mania). But this is a minor caveat in an impressive historical accomplishment perhaps worthy of future debate.

 

January 2, 2014

 

Thomas A. Ban’s reply to Barry Blackwell’s Comments

         I appreciate the comments of Dr. Blackwell and hope The Forgotten Language will find its way from our website into education. I also hope it will stimulate not only defining psychiatric populations in terms of psychopathology and psychiatric nosology but also linking these populations to abnormal brain functioning.   

         I share his views about the importance of lithium in pharmacotherapy in psychiatry. I keep lithium apart from chlorpromazine, meprobamate, iproniazid and imipramine only because it is a natural and not a synthetic substance that was rediscovered and not discovered in the same time period as those drugs.

 

January 23, 2014

 

Jose de Leon’s Comments

         This is my second review on a piece by Dr. Ban that Dr. Blackwell has commented on. To summarize my conflict of interest to them I need to acknowledge that I am neutral toward Dr. Blackwell but I am positively biased toward Dr. Ban.  This time my conclusion is the opposite of the prior review on Dr. Ban’s essay on “conflict of interest” (see Controversies, March 20, 2014); while Dr. Ban has done an excellent job, I tend to disagree with Dr. Blackwell’s perspective. 

         I need to acknowledge that I share Dr. Ban’s love for the history of psychiatry and the Heidelberg school of psychiatry which developed “descriptive psychopathology”.

         I currently think that US psychiatry has reached an impasse with the DSM-5 and RDoC (de Leon J. DSM-5 and Research Domain Criteria: One hundred years after Jaspers’s General Psychopathology. Am J  Psychiatry 2014; 171: 492-4). Moreover, this end is the result of the process started in 1972 with Feighner’s criteria (Feighner JP, Robins E, Guze SB, Woodruff RA Jr, Winokur G, Munoz R. Diagnostic criteria for use in psychiatric research. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1972;26:57-63), also known as the Neokraepelinian revolution, that led to the DSM-III (de Leon J.  One hundred years of limited impact of Jasper’s General Psychiatry in the US psychiatry. J Nerv Ment Dis. 2014; 202:79-87). However, as Andreasen described, it had the unintended consequences of killing US psychiatry’s interest in descriptive psychopathology (DSM and the death of phenomenology in America: an example of unintended consequences. Schizophr Bull. 2007; 33: 108-12). Moreover, Dr. McHugh’s warnings about DSM nosology (Striving for coherence: psychiatry's efforts over classification. JAMA 2005; 293: 2526-8) were not heeded. Dr. Blackwell has demonstrated some, but not enough enthusiasm, in my opinion, in reviewing Dr. Ban’s e-book.  I think he should have encouraged all US psychiatrists to read it. 

         Other American psychiatrists with compatible views are the before-mentioned Drs. Andreasen and McHugh.  Also, Dr. Weinberger who previously worked in the NIMH but dislikes the NIMH’s RDoC (Weinberger, DR.  RDoCs redux. World Psychiatry 2014;13:36-38).  I would emphasize that Dr. Ban does possess unique comprehensive knowledge of descriptive psychiatry which needs to be complemented by o+++thers in the field.  Dr. McHugh is ideal for teaching US psychiatrists how to diagnose, and Dr. Andreasen is ideal for teaching them how to assess psychosis and incorporate descriptive psychopathology in scales.  Although I have read many of Drs. Andreasen’s and McHugh’s articles, I do not remember them ever quoting Leonhard. Thus, Dr. Ban needs to stay active to teach US psychiatry about the Leonhardian system of psychosis diagnosis.  In my experience, 99% of US psychiatrists have no idea who Leonhard was, or what cycloid psychoses are.   Dr. Ban quotes Dr. Pichot’s book (Pichot P. A Century of Psychiatry. Paris: Roger Dacosta; 1983) in his E-book. Dr. Pichot is also an expert in the history of psychiatry and his ideas may help to reorient US psychiatry in a useful direction. .

 

May 22, 2014

 

Thomas A. Ban’s reply to Jose de Leon’s comments

         Thank you very much for you complimentary comments. It is indeed unfortunate that Karl Jaspers’ General Psychopathology and Karl Leonhard’s Classification of Endogenous Psychoses did not receive their deserved recognition. The dismissal of these contributions during the second half of the twentieth century deprived research in neuropsychopharmacology from more discriminate clinical end points in the schizophrenias and bipolar disorders than provided by diagnoses in consensus-based classifications. We hope that INHN’s educational program in the history of the field will help to rectify the situation.   

 

December 4, 2014

 

Hector Warnes’ comments

         It is a pleasure to read this monumental study on the evolution of the pivotal “forgotten language” of psychopathology and the conflicting nosologies based on semiology that have arisen in our attempts to understand mental illnesses since the 19th century.  Prof. Ban has written a panoramic and systematic view of the latest integrative discoveries of our young discipline, which cannot be independent of the neurosciences nor of multifactorial triggers, internal and external, with different degrees of importance as in most of medicine. Let   us not forget Antoine Laurent Bayle, who in 1822 discovered a medical pathology for a mental disease, which in fact, was neurosyphillis and called it chronic arachnoiditis, the cause of a “symptomatic mental alienation.” Bayle thus introduced the medical model in psychiatry. Constantin von Economo studied the sequelae of the encephalitis lethargica following the flu pandemic in 1917-1918 and described not only the neurological, but also various psychopathological sequelae (Pierre Pichot: A Century of Psychiatry. Hoffmann- La Roche: Basle Switzerland; 1983), later emphasized by Oliver Sacks in his book Awakenings. He attributed this eruption into life of severe Parkinsonian patients that had many depressive features and that were able to respond to L-dopa given by George Cotzias.  Oliver Sacks at first was skeptical about L-dopa because he was anti-reductionist and believed that there were other factors in what he called ‘a post-encephalitic disorder of far greater complexity, severity and strangeness’   (Oliver Sacks: Awakenings. Harper Perennial; 1990). Electroencephalography was introduced by a German psychiatrist Hans Berger in 1929, which later on allowed further understanding of different psychopathological symptoms manifested by EEG changes and the newer neuropharmacological therapies in the nineteen-fifties also reflected in EEG disturbances.  Ictal manifestations were known to improve psychotic states and were used by Cerletti and Bini as treatment for depression or acute functional psychosis with Electroconvulsive therapy.  What was less known was the interictal psychosis, the forced normalization of Landolt, the postictal psychosis, the paranoid and affective states, cognitive disorders, confusional states, bizarre conducts, language disturbances and dreamy states, the latter described by W. Penfield during neuro-surgical  stimulation of  the temporal lobe with micro- electrodes  (Gerhard-Schorsch: Epilepsie: Klinik und Forschung. In Psychiatrie der Gegenwart-Band II. Springer-Verlag; 1960). There are many medical diseases preceded by depression for months such as cancer of the colon, pancreas and lung and finally the dementias that have a very complex psychopathology, which may affect any area of the brain with its distinct psychopathological symptom before we can reach a clear diagnosis. We psychiatrist still have a wider field in the research of the psychiatric aftermaths of craneo-encephalic traumata that are so prevalent. For further reading on this issue, I recommend Psychiatric Aspects of Neurologic Disease, edited by D.F. Benson and D. Blumer (Grune and Stratton: New York; 1975)..

         Tom Ban citing Pierre Pichot writes: “Every mental disorder implies a disease of the nervous system, but not every defect of the nervous system is accompanied by mental disorder.” This statement sums up the above arguments.

         The introduction of experimental psychopathology thanks to the work of Pavlov, Horsley Gantt, Masserman and H.S. Lidell among others were reviewed by Ban in his book Conditioning and Psychiatry (Aldine Publishing Company: Chicago;1964). Quoting Prof. Ban in his “Forgotten language” page 8: “Hence, abnormal Conditional Reflex functions could be linked to psychopathology and its underlying abnormality; C.R. parameters would provide a bridge between the ‘language’ of mental pathology’ and the ‘language of pathological brain functioning.’”

         Charles Shagass wrote a chapter on Experimental Psychopathology in The Comprehensive Textbook of Psychiatry, edited by A.M. Freedman and H.I. Kaplan (The William and Wilkins Company; 1967). In the same book there are excellent chapters on Hallucinogens (psychotomimetics) by Arnold Mandell and Louis Jolyon West (Ban cited Moreau de Tours who first described the psychodysleptic effects of Hashish in 1845) and on the pathogenic effects of Sensory Deprivation first explored by Bexton, W.H. Heron, W. and Scott, T.H. (Effects of decreased variation in the sensory environment. Canad. J. Psychology 1954;  8: 7) and further expanded by H. Azima, R. Vispo and F.J. Azima at the Allan Memorial Institute in Montreal.

         Prof. Ban   in citing Carl Wernicke who classified psychiatric disease on the basis of hyperfunctioning, hypofunctioning or parafunctioning in the psychosensory, intrapsychic (transcortical) and psychomotor components of the psychic reflect (first noted by Griesinger)   takes us from physiology to molecular biology. Professor Ban has clarified many issues regarding Leonhard’s nosology particularly thanks to the English school (Frank Fish) and American-Hungarian school at Vanderbilt University in Nashville to end at the Hungarian school of the University of Semmelweis in Budapest where a new psychopathological classification was launched.

         Ban reminds us that the Heidelberg school first introduced the distinction between pathologies of symbolization (condensation and onomatopoesis) later taken up by Freud and pathologies of psychomotility (ambitendency and parakinesis). Ambitendency or ambivalence was taken up by E. Bleuler as one of the primary symptoms of schizophrenia.

         He further reminds us of Jaspers monumental work on psychopathology largely phenomenological in the sense of describing abnormal subjective experiences: “while nosology deals with the synthesis of disease entities from symptoms and signs and a classification of the diseases synthesized” (p. 12). As Prof. Ban stated, we have made a great leap forward since Abraham Wikler’s book, published in 1957, which emphasized the biological underpinning of mental disorders up to the latest advances in Positron Emission Tomography, Single Photon Emission Tomography and other neuroimaging techniques which support Ban’s analysis, including  molecular biology and molecular genetics.

         I would like to put forward the nosological schema proposed by Henry Ey, who was inspired by the British neurologists, C.S. Sherrington regarding the integration of the CNS and Hughlings Jackson regarding the dissolution or disintegration of higher mental functions, releasing lower levels of brain activity:

1.Pathology of consciousness: acute psychosis, manic and depressive episodes, cloudiness of sensorium, confusional psychosis, acute delirium and acute delusional states. I would add Meduna’s oneirophrenia.

2.The pathology of personality refers to psychopathies and neurosis, chronic schizophrenias, delusional psychosis and dementias (Henri Ey: Ëtudes psychiatriques- structure des psychoses aigues et déstructuration de la conscience. Volume III. Desclée de Brouwer: Paris; 1954). The “pathologie du champ de la conscience” seems to coincide in part with the “acute exogenous reaction types” of Bonhoeffer and with the “bouffées delirantes.”  A very impressive review on this issue was published by Klaus Conrad (Die symptomatischen Psychosen in Psychiatrie der Gegenwart. Band II. Springer Verlag; 1960).

         Henry Ey appears to be influenced as well by Jaspers,  who distinguished between phasic or periodic and process or development of the personality. Further reading of the most significant contributions of European psychiatry can be found in Themes and Variations of European Psychiatry edited by S.R. Hirsch and M. Shepherd (University Press of Virginia: Charlottesville; 1974). In this book, there is a chapter on Patterns (Die Erscheinungsformen) of mental disorders written by E. Kraepelin, who in supporting Birnbaum’s view, states: “We may say that the pathognomonic disturbances -those which unfailingly appear in the same illness- are produced directly by the basic underlying cause, while the less constant phenomena, which appear in varying guise or which may be altogether absent, are attributable to factors personal to the patient” (p.16). As you recall, Birnbaum differentiated between pathogenic and pathoplastic factors.

         Karl Leonard classification was validated by Frank Fish (1964). Between 79% of 123 unsystematic schizophrenics and 84.4% of 51 cases of affect-laden paraphrenics responded to phenothiazines while only 23 % of 311 systematic schizophrenics and 23 % of 100 systematic hebephrenics responded to the same drug (page 20).

         In another study by Guy, Ban and Wilson (1985), 4.3 % of unsystematic schizophrenics developed tardive dyskinesia while 13.3 % of systematic schizophrenics had this complication.

         Regarding the Composite Diagnostic Evaluation (CODE), which showed a high inter-rater reliability, was compared with the DSM-III and Ban noted that the latter “reified pharmacologically heterogenous diagnostic concepts” implying that was far from a-theoretical.

         The CODE-DD was adopted to fit neuropsychopharmacological research. Ban found that of 322 patients with major depression, only 37% were validated using the CODE-DD.

         An original “structural psychopathology” was proposed by Gyula Nyirö (1958, 1962) from Budapest. The structures were three: afferent cognitive, central affective and efferent adaptive ones. Ban proposes a “nosological homotyping” which would “provide a potential diagnostic concept for a mental disorder in which psychopathological symptoms represent distinct abnormalities in the processing of signals between levels within and across these  three mental structures” (p. 28),  taking into account the onset, the course and the dynamic totality of the pathological process. For Ban, the nosological homotyping would allow pharmacological validation.

         In this complex E-book, Ban includes in the validation studies the latest development of molecular biology such as G protein coupled receptors, nuclear hormonal receptors, ion channels, enzymes, and so on (p, 26), which using a treatment responsive population to a psychotropic drug could serve as a reference point for testable genetic hypotheses (p.26). Since the discovery of the role of neurotransmitters alterations in schizophrenia and major depression, we have come a long way because it would appear that the substratum of mental illnesses is far more complex. Unfortunately, even reading Ban’s paper, which is far from being reductionist, we cannot yet follow an Occam’s razor argument: entia non sunt multiplicanda praetor necessitate (entities are not to multiplied beyond necessity) (Simon Blackburn: Oxford Dictionary of Philosophy. Oxford University Press; 1996).

         Ban’s E-Book is an overview of the developments of psychopathology and nosology, matching each syndrome or cluster of sign and symptoms with the latest advances in neuropsychopharmacology and the neurosciences.  He has gone with great originality into sorting out disease entities by “nosological homotyping,” which would take into account the genome and the environment. The existence of signals of facilitation and suppression in the cortex are well established as the role of different brain circuits in most mental disorders.

         I am inclined to suggest that the glia has as much importance as the neurone per se and that the validation of the DSM-5 or that of the Leonhard nosology is far from completed because of limitations in our understanding of the basic substratum of many mental disorders.

 

March 26, 2015

 

Thomas A. Ban’s reply to Hector Warnes’ comments

         I appreciate Professor Warnes’ comments on my E-Book. With his impressive knowledge, he touches upon contributions from Antoine Bayle to Charles Shagass with Economo, Hans Berger, Landoll, Penfield, Wernicke, Pavlov, Gantt, Masserman and Lidell in between; expresses concerns about conflicting nosologies based on semiology; puts forward Henry Ey’s “nosological scheme”; points out the lack of validation of DSM-5 and Leonhard’s classification; and shares his belief that in the brain glial cells are as much of importance as neurons. It is unfortunate that by extending the scope of his review, and especially by taking Bayle’s contributions as starting point, he misses a development that lead from Griesinger’s adoption of reflex activity as the basis of all brain activity in the 1840s to the proposition in the early 21st century that “nosological homotyping” might provide suitable populations for studying the relationship between mental and neuronal activity. Although in his five pages scholarly comment, he dedicates a paragraph to “nosological homotyping,” he evades to address the central theme of “Forgotten Language” that psychiatric patients based on differences in their abnormal forms (and not content) of their experiences, grouped on the basis of nosological organizing principles (and not diagnoses), provide more homogenous psychiatric populations than psychiatric populations based on any other criteria.

 

May 14, 2015

 

Hector Warnes’ response to Thomas A. Ban’s reply

         Really, I am not qualified to comment on a paper which is written not only by an expert in conditioning reflexes and the neural arc but by a seasoned researcher on the key psychoneuropharmacology issues since the fifties.  I understand now that his long preamble was with the intention to amass a working hypothesis, “the missing link” regarding the selection of  a homogeneous  population of patients with specific psychopathology  that could be assigned to the same “nosological matrix based on distinct abnormalities in the processing of signals between levels,  within and across three mental structures: type of onset, course of the disease and outcome which would represent the pathological process in its dynamic totality” (p. 50). The latter statements support his view that nosology must be considered in its totality, temporality and polarity, each with its own variations.  This fine discrimination is not taken into account in most neuropsychopharmacology research. Neuronal circuits that respond to CRs and nosological homotyping “might provide the missing link for using abnormality of CR parameters for bridging psychopathology and pathophysiology in the CNS” (p. 50). Prof. Ban’s Project might seem utopic but worth pursuing.  In my experience, I am often surprised by the unpredictability of the outcome of most psychiatric illnesses, the frequency of co-morbidity, overlapping of symptoms and epigenetic surprises just around the corner.  It is true that most research is limited in time span and the follow up of patients do not take into account Prof. Ban’s  search for more precision and specificity in the best tradition of Wernicke and Kleist regarding brain localization (neuro-anatomy) of specific psychopathological manifestations and further, in the tradition of Pavlov,  Prof. Ban’s  goal is to find specific pathophysiological circuits up to the biological molecular level that would be the target for drug-action

         The course and outcome of many disorders in the wider field of medicine such as Rheumatoid Arthritis, Multiple Sclerosis, Lupus,  Hypertension, Vascular disease and so on have a spectrum of minor manifestations to severe ones and an outcome that may be considered favourable and others most unfavourable in spite of all the best treatments available up to date. In the earlier stages of most psychiatric disorders we are faced with similar issues. But in later stages when the disease process has been established and has crystallized beyond doubt irreversibility and even deterioration sets in. Then we may wonder if the condition at hand is due to the adverse environment, to improper treatment or to the course of the disease per se.

         I do hope that we should be able to find homogeneous populations in order to validate the selection of patients in a personalized way for specific pharmacotherapeutic agents in the light of molecular biology and accurate psychopathological assessment. Nosological homotyping might rule out certain pathoplastic factors and be in a condition to search for more precise pathogenic factors in Birnbaum’s conception   As the decades go by, the boundaries between several psychiatric entities are getting more blurred, particularly those associated with personality disorders at the point that we get bogged down in our purely scientific task regarding the specificity and validity of long term prognosis. I am impressed by the achievements of Karl Leonhard and the Hungarian School in their search for more accurate predictions based on follow-up studies of distinct psychopathological entities. We already have the tools to correlate this distinct psychopathological structures with more precise brain circuits. Hence there is hope for nosological homotyping

 

July 23, 2015

 

Thomas Ban’s response to Hector Warnes’ response

         This short essay does not have a “lengthy preamble to amass to a working hypothesis,” as Professor Warnes suggests, but a sketchy outline of history that led to the establishment of a foundation for psychiatry in psychopathology and nosology. In reviewing history, it simply shows that during the second part of the 20th century these two disciplines were abandoned (“Forgotten”). Hence, it suggests “nosologic homotyping” to derive populations exclusively based on psychopathology and psychiatric nosology to be able to test the usefulness of these two disciplines before they are summarily dismissed. Since in his response to my reply to his comment Professor Warnes inadvertently confounded what nosologic homotyping is and nosologic homotypes are, in the following, both concepts will be briefly defined.

         Nosologic homotyping is based on structural psychopathology, a branch of psychopathology in which Wernicke’s “mental structure” is combined with 20th century psychopathology. Thus, in structural psychopathology the three components of Wernicke’s “psychic structure” (psychosensory, intrapsychic and psychomotor) are extended into three “mental structures,” the “afferent-cognitive,” the “central-affective” and the “efferent–adaptive.” Nosologic homotypes are identical in “elementary units”  of mental illness (psychopathological symptoms) and are assigned to the same position in the “nosologic matrix,” constructed with consideration of the three classifying principles of psychiatric nosology, i.e., “totality, (“universal” or “partial”), “temporality” (“continuous” or “episodic”) and “polarity” (“bipolar” or “unipolar”). Each pharmacologically valid distinct “nosologic homotype” provides a potential diagnostic concept for a mental disorder.

 

July 30, 2015

 

August 8, 2019


{1} In his First Lines of the Practice of Physic, Cullen characterized the “class” of  “neuroses”  by “injury of sense and motion without an  idiopathic pyrexia or any local affliction”, and divided the “neuroses” into four “orders” of diseases: “comata” (“diminution of  voluntary motion with sleep or deprivation of senses”), “adinamiae”( diminution of involuntary motions whether vital or natural”), “spasmi” (“irregular motions of the muscles or muscle fibres”) and  “vesaniae”  (disorders of judgment without pyrexia or coma”) (Menninger, Mayman and Pruyser 1968).  

{2} Within Pavlov’s frame of reference, CR disinhibition is a function of “external inhibition, whereas CR extinction, CR differentiation and CR delay (retardation) are  functions of “internal inhibition.

{3} Major European developments in psychopathology and psychiatric nosology during the first half of the 20th century were translated only many years after the introduction of psychotropic drugs. Karl Jaspers’ General Psychopathology published in 1913 was first translated into English in 1962, and Karl Leonhard’s Classification of Endogenous Psychoses published in 1957 was first translated in 1979.The psychometric  methodology that dominated psychopharmacology during the second part of the 20th century was developed without the possible benefit of those contributions.

{4} From the 10 members of the team 4 were to become professors and heads of university departments of psychiatry: Eugenio Aguglia in Trieste (Italy), Istvan Bitter in Budapest (Hungary), François Ferrero in Geneva (Switzerland) and Marek Jarema in Warsaw (Poland). .